Skip to main content

Exploratory mixed methods analysis of self-authored content from participants in a digital alcohol intervention trial

Abstract

Background

Digital interventions readily permit data capture of participant engagement with them. If future interventions are intended to be more interactive, tailored, or a useful resource offered to users, it may be valuable to examine such data. One module available in a digital alcohol intervention recently tested in a randomised control trial offered participants the opportunity to self-author prompts that were sent to them by a text message at a time of their choosing. This study thus aimed to evaluate these self-authored prompts to increase knowledge on how individuals negotiate behaviour change and assess whether intervention content can be improved in the future.

Methods

The self-authored prompts were evaluated qualitatively using a combination of content and thematic analysis. The identified themes and subcategories are exemplified using anonymized quotes, and the frequency that each identified theme was coded for among the prompts was calculated. Associations between baseline characteristics and the odds of authoring a prompt at all, as well as a prompt within each theme, were investigated using logistic regression.

Results

Five themes were identified (Encouragement Style, Level of Awareness, Reminders of reasons to reduce/quit, Strategies to reduce/quit, and Timescale), all with several subcategories. The prompts module was more likely to be used by women and older individuals, as well as those for whom reducing alcohol consumption was perceived as important, or who felt they had the know-how to do so. Participants who had immediate access to the support tool (intervention group) were more than twice as likely to author a prompt (OR = 2.36; probability of association > 99%) compared to those with 4-month delayed access (control group).

Conclusions

Individuals who engaged with the prompts module showed evidence of using the information provided in the support tool in an active way, with several showing goal setting and making plans to change their drinking behaviour. Individuals also used this opportunity to remind themselves of personal and specific reasons they wanted to change their drinking, as well as to encourage themselves to do so.

Background

Regular consumption of alcohol is associated with an increased risk of negative physical and psychological consequences, as well as harm to others and society more broadly[1,2,3]. Behavioural change interventions are one option for supporting individuals to reduce their alcohol consumption [4]. In countries where internet connectivity and mobile phone ownership are widespread, interventions can be disseminated digitally, reducing pressure on primary healthcare to provide such support. Although the evidence supporting digital alcohol interventions in treatment of excess alcohol consumption is not yet conclusive [5, 6], reviews indicate that such interventions can lead to reductions in total weekly alcohol consumption and episodes of heavy drinking [7]. Offering alcohol interventions digitally could also confer additional benefits such as increased accessibility and reduced stigma [8]. Nonetheless, there is room for improvement regarding the effectiveness of digital interventions, which can be negatively impacted by factors such as risk of attrition [6]. Digital interventions differ in their design and content, as well as how this content is delivered, and features and modules vary in their perceived usefulness and satisfaction among users [9]. A common feature is the use of prompts—reminders or advice that are sent by email, text message, or push notification—which aim to support behaviour change through providing motivational messages to increase self-efficacy, and tips to increase knowledge of how to change.

A digital support tool was recently developed and tested in Sweden by Bendtsen and colleagues [5, 10] in a randomized control trial (RCT) [10]. There, 2129 individuals with harmful alcohol use (aged 18 + years and with access to a mobile phone) were randomized to either a control (N = 1066) or intervention (N = 1063) group. The results [10] indicated that the intervention group reported reduced alcohol consumption relative to the control group at both 2- and 4-month follow-up in terms of total weekly consumption (incidence rate ratio (IRR) = 0.89) and heavy episodic drinking (IRR = 0.77). The tested support tool [5, 10, 11] included a module where participants were invited to self-author prompts that they may find useful. This optional module (hereafter referred to as the prompts module) took the form of writing a text message to themselves, which was then delivered on a date and time of their choosing in the coming week (up to three times). This could for instance be a reminder of their desire to change or a plan for dealing with a particular situation that could be anticipated in advance. Although this was, to the best knowledge of the authors, a novel module, it was hypothesised that it would leverage behavioural change techniques [11] relating to problem solving and goal setting [12] for which there is evidence that their inclusion in interventions may support behavioural change. Specifically, participants were prompted to 1) write messages that reminded them of their commitment and 2) have these sent to themselves at times when they anticipated that there may be environmental triggers to drink. Thus, the intention behind the prompts module was to offer participants a resource in planning for coping with predictable behavioural triggers, encourage problem solving, and reinforce earlier intentions [5].

Here, we present an exploratory analysis of the self-authored prompts collected during the parent trial [10, 12], whose content may offer insights into how individuals’ express their motivations, readiness to change, self-reflection, and the various strategies they may employ to reduce their drinking. Such insight could indicate how well intervention content reflects how participants negotiate behavioural change with themselves and highlight whether the content and information provided is in alignment with what those trying to reduce their drinking focus on and reflect over. This may be relevant for intervention design because recent additional analysis of data from the same parent trial suggests that study participation can improve know-how and confidence to reduce alcohol consumption and that this mediates behavioural change [13]. Previous work investigating user-experiences among the intervention group in another RCT targeting alcohol consumption also indicated that self-reflection over their drinking appeared to be an important driver of reduced consumption [14]. As such, designing interventions that contribute to or facilitate a boost in confidence and know-how or induce self-reflection is of relevance. The primary objective of the present work was thus to qualitatively investigate the content of self-authored prompts to obtain insight into such self-reflection, and general negotiation of behavioural change as a process, as described by individuals themselves during a trial.

Further, if alcohol interventions are to become more individualized and tailored in the future, as has been proposed as a possible way to improve effectiveness [15], assessing for whom given intervention content, e.g., a prompts module, is appealing to would be fruitful. A secondary objective was thus to test whether baseline characteristics were associated with the likelihood of participants engaging with the prompts module at all, or were related to the types of prompts that were written.

As a waiting-list design was employed in the parent trial, the possible implications of this for engagement with the prompts module as well as the types of prompts authored were also explored (i.e., contrasts between the intervention group who were given immediate access to the support tool and the control group who waited four months before receiving access to it were explored). This was deemed of interest as the implications of waiting-list designs are not yet fully understood [12, 16] although it has been suggested that individuals with increased readiness to change may be particularly negatively impacted by being put on a waiting list for support tool access [17]. Variations in how the intervention and control group engaged with the prompts module may elucidate ways in which a waiting list design affects how those made to wait may engage with support tools they are subsequently given access to, beyond assessing drinking behaviour alone.

Method

Trial design

The parent study was a two-arm, double-blind, parallel group randomised effectiveness trial of a digital alcohol intervention. The trial was prospectively registered (ISRCTN48317451) and a trial protocol including a statistical analysis plan was made available prior to trial commencement [5]. Full methodological and intervention design details are provided in the Appendix.

Participants

The target population of the main trial was Swedish adults seeking help online to reduce their alcohol consumption. Individuals were required to be at least 18 years of age, have access to a mobile phone, and be classified as having harmful alcohol use according to Swedish guidelines. At the time of data collection, this was defined as either drinking more than 9 (female) or 14 (male) standard drinks of alcohol per week (total weekly consumption) or drinking more than 4 (female) or 5 (male) standard drinks on a single occasion at least once a month (heavy episodic drinking). A standard drink is in Sweden defined as 12 g of alcohol. All study materials were in Swedish, and so individuals who did not comprehend Swedish well enough to understand these were implicitly excluded.

Participants were recruited to the trial using web search engine advertisements (Google, Yahoo, Bing) and Facebook. Individuals interested in the study sent a text message to a dedicated phone number. Within 10 min, they received a text message with a hyperlink that took them to a Web page asking for informed consent. Those who consented were asked to respond to a baseline questionnaire (which also assessed eligibility). The questionnaire included questions on demographics, current alcohol consumption, and three single item measures of confidence in one’s ability to reduce drinking (confidence), perceived importance of reducing drinking (importance), and knowledge of how to reduce drinking (know-how). Eligible participants were randomised after responding to the baseline questionnaire to either receive the novel digital alcohol intervention or to a control group. Drinking behaviour (weekly consumption and episodes of heavy drinking) was measured at 2-month and 4-month follow-up. The control group were provided with information regarding the risks associated with alcohol consumption and informed that they would receive access to the support tool after four months (waiting list design). There were no other differences between the control and intervention groups apart from the control group being given access to the novel intervention following completion of the 4-month follow-up. The demographics of the full cohort and those who engaged with the prompts module (N = 476 respondents) within each group (intervention and control) are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of total participants in the intervention and control groups, and those who used the prompts module

Outcomes

One component of the intervention was to offer participants the opportunity to author their own prompts that they would receive at a time and date of their own choosing (up to three times a week). This could for instance be a reminder of their desire to change their behaviour or a plan for when they are confronted with a behavioural trigger. Authoring prompts was an optional component of the intervention. For the present work, the self-authored prompts themselves are a primary outcome, in the form of written qualitative data. Whether or not participants authored any prompts while having access to the intervention tool was a secondary outcome. In total, 1166 prompts were authored.

Data analysis

It should be acknowledged that all analyses herein were exploratory and were not described in the trial protocol. This was due to the researchers not anticipating that the prompts module would be so frequently used as it turned out to be.

Qualitative analysis

The qualitative approach taken is best described as a combination of content and thematic analysis [18]. Aspects of content analysis that were relevant include describing the characteristics of the prompts based on the actual words used and the frequency that similar terms appear, as well as the identification of recurring keywords. The positioning of content analysis as appropriate for exploring relatively unknown phenomena [18] meant that this approach was more relevant in the earlier steps of the analysis. Aspects of thematic analysis that were employed were assessing how the codes grouped into broader concepts (based both on manifest content and behavioural theory), and extrapolation of underlying concepts that were not necessarily explicitly stated. This approach was more relevant in the later steps of the analysis. The qualitative analysis was conducted on text-only data in the original written language and coders were blinded to randomization status.

The prompts were read and re-read by a single researcher (ESC), who generated notes and an initial code list and codebook. The initial codebook was reviewed by a second researcher who was familiar with the dataset (MB), for clarity and comprehensiveness. The data were then coded in RQDA (R package for Qualitative Data Analysis [19]) according to the initial codebook. There was no limit on the number of codes for a single prompt. During this stage, annotations and notes pertaining to ways that the codes could potentially be grouped based on both semantic meaning and patterns of applying the codes (e.g., recurring instances of codes clustering together) were made. Some codes were merged, some were discarded, and new codes were added. The codebook was updated accordingly and reviewed alongside the fully coded dataset by MB.

After a joint discussion on the codebook and code applications, where further adjustments and clarifications were made, the codes were conceptually grouped which gave rise to an initial list of candidate themes. Codes that did not seem to fit within these groupings were retained during the next round of coding. During this step, the coding process was more carefully guided by attempting to identify broader concepts associated with capability, opportunity, and motivation, as well as intentions and evidence of behavioural planning and application of strategies for alcohol reduction. The codebook was again refined, including renaming, merging, and removing codes (particularly those which did not appear to fit with any of the wider identified concepts due to being highly specific, or not substantially present throughout the dataset). Prompts which remained uncoded at this stage were not considered further in the analysis (N = 88).

The codebook was then used by a third researcher (JB) to independently code the full dataset. Following this, based on discrepancies in coding between coders, further codebook refinements were made and the dataset was coded an additional time jointly ESC and JB. The final coded dataset was assessed and agreed upon by the researchers. The codebook is provided in the Appendix, Table A1.

Quantitative analysis

To transform the qualitative data into a form suitable for quantitative processing, the presence (1) or absence (0) of each code for every prompt was recorded. Thereafter, the presence or absence of each thematic category was noted for each participant depending on the subcategories assigned to all the prompts they had authored.

Logistic regression was used to assess if any baseline characteristics (age, gender, household characteristics, weekly alcohol consumption, episodes of heavy drinking, confidence, importance, know-how) as well as group allocation (intervention vs. control) were associated with an increased odds of engaging with the prompts module. Logistic regression was also used to estimate the odds ratio of authoring a prompt belonging to a specific thematic category given baseline characteristics or group allocation. Models were estimated using the quap function from the rethinking package [20] in R (version 4.1.1). Standard normal priors (µ = 0; σ = 1) were used for all coefficients.

Results

As shown in Fig. 1, most respondents wrote 1–3 prompts, with 3 being the most common, while very few authored four or more prompts while having access to the support tool.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Shows the percentage of respondents who authored one, two, three, and four or more prompts while having access to the support tool

Thematic content of self-authored prompts

As shown in Table 2, the most frequently coded for subcategory was Health - general intention/desire to improve physical or emotional well-being, whilst the least frequently coded for subcategories were money and longer-term thinking. Five thematic categories (themes) were identified the dataset, each with subcategories, see Table 2. The frequency that each subcategory appeared in both the control and intervention group is shown in Fig. 2.

Table 2 Thematic categories and subcategories detected through qualitative analysis and the total frequency (%) of prompts in each. Note: Percentages are rounded and do not sum to exactly 100%.
Fig. 2
figure 2

Diagram showing the frequency of codes appearing in the intervention and control groups

In the sections that follow, the thematic categories are described and explored, with anonymized quotes provided to exemplify these and their associated subcategories.

Thematic category 1: encouragement styles

The category Encouragement Style encompasses instances where respondents were imparting some degree of supportive message or call to action to themselves. Three subcategories were identified: Self-Control (urging themselves to retain or take control over their alcohol consumption), Self-Care (urging themselves to put themselves first, to be kind and gentle on themselves, or take care of their own physical/mental state), and Pride/Self-Belief (displaying that they are or should be proud of themselves, or that they believe in their ability to reduce their drinking). Overall, Self-Control was more prevalent than the other two encouragement styles, and both Self-Control and Pride/Self-Belief were more common among the intervention group than the control group. The three Encouragement Styles are exemplified below in descending order of frequency (Self-Control, Pride/Self-Belief, Self-Care). The Encouragement Styles were sometimes co-coded, in particular Self-Control and Pride/Self-belief.

“Avoid [alcohol] today, Thursday!!! It is NOT worth the tiredness during Friday’s workday. Keep going!”

“Fight for my goal to lose weight, dare to change my patterns. I can and I will”

“You feel better today right? Nice to avoid anxiety and being drunk from spirits, right? Take care of yourself!”

Thematic category 2: level of awareness

Many participants wrote prompts conveying some Level of Awareness regarding the impact their drinking has had or is having on their life or others around them. Two subcategories were identified: Broad Awareness and Specified Consequences. The key difference between these is that Broad Awareness indicates a general sense of negativity towards their current behaviour or awareness that there can be consequences to drinking excessively, whilst Specific Consequences involves specifying some direct impact of drinking on other decisions and behaviours (e.g., sending texts that are regretted later), goals (e.g., getting fitter or losing weight), and people (e.g. negatively impacting a relationship), or stating some direct consequence of drinking (e.g., poorer performance at work). Broad Awareness was coded for more often than Specified Consequences overall. The representation of Broad Awareness was higher among the intervention than control group, whilst Specified Consequences appeared at a similar frequency in both groups. Although prompts coded as Broad Awareness ranged from short reflections on their behaviour to lengthier consideration of the reasons they drink or implications that they are dissatisfied with their current circumstances, they lacked specificity. Specified Consequences could in some ways be considered the next step succeeding Broad Awareness. Nonetheless, it seems reasonable that—akin to individuals typically moving along the stages-of-change in a non-linear pattern—there would be some iterative movement between the two, and it should be emphasized that the self-authored prompts here provide only snapshots of respondents’ reasoning. Examples of Broad Awareness and Specified Consequences, respectively, are:

“It’s ok! You don’t need alcohol to reward yourself or to drink away the difficult things!”

“Remember not to drink too much. It leads to bad decisions and damages relationships”

Prompts coded as Broad Awareness were not coded alongside codes within the Strategies category often but this did occur, typically when the prompt consisted of multiple sentences. For example, the following prompts were coded as Broad Awareness alongside Limit Opportunity and Strategy – Units respectively.

“Don’t go to the off-licence! You’ll see what a difference there is after a week without alcohol.”

“Buy max 4 beers at the off-licence this afternoon! It’s time to take things in hand now!”

Like Broad Awareness, Specified Consequences also sometimes appeared alongside codes from Strategies. However, it was more common that Reasons were co-coded here. This is logical: specifying the consequences of drinking requires overtly recognizing other behaviours or individuals that are impacted by the person’s drinking. The most common Reasons coded alongside Specified Consequences were Health (both avoid negative consequences and approach positive outcomes), Mental Health, and Other People, exemplified respectively here:

“Get rid of thoughts about reward or stress. It only leads to anxiety and bad text messages.”

“Alcohol gives you unbearable anxiety. It’s not worth it, you know that.”

“You will stop drinking for your own well-being and for your kids’ sake. They don’t like seeing you unsober.”

Thematic category 3: reasons

Respondents often used the prompts module to remind themselves of specific Reasons they wanted to change their drinking behaviour. Such prompts were often short, although in some cases respondents provided a list of several reasons. The most common reasons provided were related to health, with those conveying a desire to move towards improved health (Health – Approach Positive) being more prevalent than those mentioning avoidance of negative health consequences associated with alcohol consumption (Health – Avoid Negative). Reasons relating to money and physical appearance were the least common. The Health – Approach Positive subcategory includes mentions of desiring both better physical health and general well-being, e.g., when respondents referenced wanting to “feel better” more generally. Similarly, the Health – Avoid Negative subcategory includes prompts where respondents indicated wanting to avoid poor health outcomes and negative emotions. Allusions to general wellbeing were therefore not coded as Mental Health related reasons unless specific mental health concerns were mentioned (e.g., anxiety, depression) or the term “mental health” was explicitly used by respondents. Examples of Health – Approach Positive and Health – Avoid Negative are:

“I want to feel better, have more energy, and get better health.”

“Don’t drink! Remember what happens and how damn bad you feel. Find something else! Go and train!”

Thematic category 4: strategy/goal

Several prompts suggested that respondents were already or were planning to employ some form of Strategy/goal to moderate their drinking. This took many forms, including Limit Opportunity for drinking (avoiding the pub, not keeping alcohol at home), moderating consumption by limiting or counting Days, Units, and/or Time, suggesting an Alternative to Drinking (such as taking a walk or some other activity), and Substitutions with Alcohol Replacement (including non-alcoholic versions of drinks, and water), as well as Non-Specific Goals where no specific limitations were provided but respondents seemed nonetheless to have some goal in mind. Among these, attempting to limit the number of Units consumed or setting a Non-specific Goal were the most commonly observed, whilst Limit Opportunity was the least common. Suggesting an Alternative to Drinking and mentioning a Non-Specific Goal were more common among the intervention group whilst setting goals pertaining to Units and Time and using Substitutes for alcohol, were more common among the control group. It should be noted that stating a Non-specific Goal does not necessarily mean that these individuals did not have a concrete goal, only that they did not specify it in their prompts. Examples of Non-Specific Goal, moderation through limiting Units, and suggesting an Alternative to Drinking are:

“Now you have to think about how many glasses you will have today.”

“Remember! Max two glasses per day, to feel better and avoid negative effects.”

“Take a long walk, then a cup of tea and knitting! Tomorrow is a new day! Feel well!!”

Thematic category 5: Timeline

Regarding the Timeline category, there was a clear preference among both groups to focus on the short-term rather than the long-term, and prompts coded as Short-term Thinking were also observed more often among the control group. Many prompts coded as Short-term Thinking involved participants reminding themselves to just not drink that day and were typically quite short. Although not many prompts were coded as Long-term Thinking, most of those that were came from the intervention group, possibly reflecting a more exploratory, less focused approach to change upon being given immediate access to the tool.

“Uh huh, so it’s Friday again. Think how lovely it would be to wake up on Saturday without a hangover.”

“Think about the rest of your life.”

Associations between baseline characteristics and self-authored content

The odds ratios of whether a participant authored any prompt, as well as whether they authored prompts belonging to each thematic category, are summarized in Table 3. The analysis suggests that those in the intervention group were more likely to use the prompts module (29.9% of intervention group participants) than those in the control group (14.8% of control group participants). Prompts coded as Strategies, Reasons, Timescale, and, to a lesser degree, Level of Awareness were more likely to be authored by the control group than the intervention group, whilst the intervention group seemed slightly more likely to author prompts coded as an Encouragement Style. Women and older individuals appeared more likely to engage with the prompts module as well as individuals with higher baseline importance and know-how scores. Living with children (with or without a partner) also showed an increased odds of writing a prompt relative to living alone without children. Baseline drinking metrics did not substantially affect the odds of authoring a prompt, although higher episodes of heavy drinking did seem related to somewhat higher odds of authoring prompts categorized as Awareness or Reasons as well as decreased odds for prompts categorized as a Strategy.

Table 3 Medians of the posterior distributions of odds ratio (with 2.5% and 97.% percentiles) indicating associations with the odds of authoring any prompt, and prompts that fall into each thematic group, alongside the posterior probability of association

Discussion

Insights on how individuals trying to reduce their alcohol consumption negotiate this behavioural change can be readily collected during digital intervention trials through, for example, modules inviting individuals to write prompts for their future selves. Such insight can be useful for improving intervention design by highlighting (mis)alignment in priorities (e.g., health vs. financial motivations), differences in favoured goal direction (e.g., approach positive vs. avoid negative consequences) and even how these vary by demographic factors. Such a prompts module was included in a digital support tool, the effectiveness of which was recently tested in a recent RCT conducted in Sweden [5, 10]. Through content and thematic analysis of the 1166 prompts that were self-authored by trial participants, we attempted to gain novel insights into these individuals’ engagement with their own behavioural change process.

Quantitative analysis of the factors associated with the odds of writing a prompt at all, as well as a prompt belonging to one of the five identified themes, provided data on who authored prompts as well as the frequency of different types of content. The patterns observed and how the results can contribute to future intervention design are discussed below. Because the data and approach are novel, there are limited comparisons possible with existing literature. The importance of reinforcing motivation to continue to pursue the behaviour change involved in reducing drinking, as well as the potential important role of reflection upon one’s own behaviour [14] have, however, been observed in previous studies [21].

Although engagement with the prompts module was not especially high, women and individuals living with children (with or without a partner), and older individuals were more likely to use this module. Interventions targeting these populations may therefore benefit from offering individuals the possibility to write their own prompts or keep a record of their experiences. Those who rated themselves as having higher know-how on reducing their alcohol consumption were also more likely to write a prompt, suggesting that this module may also be more attractive to those who already know what they may want to write, or what could help them change their behaviour. By extension, individuals with lower baseline know-how may be more reliant on the advice and strategies suggested within the support tool. Higher baseline know-how individuals were also more likely to author a prompt giving reasons to change but less likely to write one indicating the use of a strategy or setting a goal. While this could be interpreted counterintuitively, it seems equally possible that these individuals did not feel the need to specify their strategy for drinking less and were instead using the prompts module to remind themselves why they want to change. Individuals for whom reducing their drinking was important were also more likely to write a prompt relating to the reasons they want to change their behaviour, perhaps suggesting that this module had the added benefit of allowing them to remind themselves of the personal reasons why they perceive change as important.

It was observed that, as well being more likely to write a prompt at all, the intervention group were more likely to author prompts conveying pride/self-belief and self-control than the control group, whilst individuals in the control group were more likely to write a prompt suggesting goal setting or implementing a specific strategy to drink less. More diffuse subcategories (e.g., Broad Awareness and Non-specific goal) were also more common among the intervention than control group. These results could imply that there may be differences between groups both in terms of their status regarding behavioural change and how they experienced the support tool. Such differences are prima facie surprising, as the only difference across groups lies in being made to wait as a result of the study design (waiting-list design). By definition, all participants in the main RCT would fall somewhere between the contemplation and action stages of change, which are characterized by higher ambivalence (mental discomfort resulting from holding both positive and negative feelings towards some behaviour) relative to the pre-contemplation and maintenance stages [22]. However, the intervention group were immediately offered a tool designed to help guide them to resolving their ambivalence by reducing or eschewing drinking, whilst the control group were not. Since heightened ambivalence can increase people’s level of information processing [22] and attention paid to information relevant to resolving their discomfort [23], this may have implications for how the support tool was experienced and used by the control group upon gaining access, as well as their behaviour during the 4-month waiting period.

The experiences of the control group from the parent trial with which this work is associated are reported in detail elsewhere [16], and revealed that several (42%) reported feeling frustrated, irritated, or disappointed to be on the waiting-list (due to feeling ready to start changing). Nonetheless, 55% of respondents also reported deciding to motivate themselves to reduce their drinking despite being on a waiting list, and an additional 16% reported finding other support to help reduce their drinking [16]. This suggests that, during the waiting time, at least some individuals in the control group investigated strategies for reducing their drinking, possibly motivated by the need to resolve their ambivalence. These strategies may have later been revealed in the prompts they authored, especially given the additional time this group had to find strategies that were helpful for them. On the other hand, others in the control group may have resolved their ambivalence by drinking. Indeed, around 17% of control group participants reported deciding to continue drinking as usual until they gained access to the support tool, and 11% said that they gave up on reducing their drinking [16], lending support to this possibility. However, without measuring ambivalence, this explanation remains speculative.

Nonetheless, this issue highlights the relevance of understanding how individuals are affected by the information provided to them in a digital intervention setting. Specifically, the importance of providing information that helps people resolve their ambivalence in favour of moderating/avoiding alcohol consumption instead of inadvertently facilitating rationalising its consumption (or pushing individuals to behave defensively or feel discouraged) is emphasized. This is pertinent because, as noted above, individuals with lower know-how in changing their drinking habits may be more reliant on the information and advice provided in an intervention setting. For both groups here, approaching positive health goals was the most common Reason coded for – more so even than avoiding negative health outcomes due to drinking. This would seem to suggest that interventions using prompts may wish to find a good balance between positive and negative health messaging, instead of focusing too heavily on avoiding negative health outcomes. This may be especially pertinent as health messages that highlight the negative health outcomes can be minimised or ignored altogether [24]. This may be due to ambivalence towards the negative outcomes, which can be rationalised as an expected part of a drinking episode or the ‘price of a good time’ [25,26,27].

It is interesting that Limit Opportunity was the least common strategy detected. This illustrates a preference for moderation strategies that require exerting agency and self-control (e.g., moderation through goal setting) over changing the environment (e.g., not keeping alcohol at home or avoiding social situations where drinking would occur). Since an intended purpose of the prompts module was to encourage individuals to exercise agency, this may not be surprising. Nonetheless, participants in alcohol reduction trials can react poorly to the idea that they drink excessively [28], a notion which suggesting changes in the environment to avoid alcohol completely may reinforce by implying that the individual has little control over their drinking behaviour. As such, moderation strategies where individuals feel that they can still drink, but not to excess, may induce less mental discomfort and remain more appealing than those which remove the option entirely.

Although baseline drinking metrics did not seem to markedly affect the odds of authoring a prompt, more frequent episodes of heavy drinking were somewhat related to greater odds of authoring a prompt categorized under Awareness or Reasons, and lower odds of writing a prompt categorized as a Strategy. This may indicate that individuals with different patterns of drinking may need different types of support and have different aspects to their motivations for change. For example, advice relating to reminders of the acute negative consequences of alcohol consumption may be more useful for an individual seeking help with heavy episodic drinking behaviour but who otherwise does not drink regularly. Whereas, for an individual who consumes alcohol regularly and in a more consistent pattern, providing specific reduction strategies and daily/weekly limits may be more appealing. Tailoring the content and frequency of prompts according to baseline drinking patterns may improve intervention effectiveness [29], and this study suggests that allowing individuals to author their own prompts could offer a more potent means to achieving this. This could also contribute to the shift towards attempting to offer increased individualization in intervention design, thought to be appealing to users as well as potentially improve efficacy [15, 29], in line with the wider literature on adaptive designs [30].

Limitations

That a large proportion (77.6%) of participants did not author prompts highlights that we cannot be sure that the dataset we obtained is representative of all participants from the parent trial. The risk of selection bias is an important study limitation because, in addition to the observed differences, those who chose to use the prompts module may be different from those who did not in unmeasured ways. It should also be noted that individuals in the control group represented here are likely to be those who are highly motivated to change their drinking behaviour, since they engaged with the support tool even after being on a waiting list for 4 months.

Some of the prompts were single words and somewhat difficult to code due to lack of context: many of the 88 prompts that remained uncoded were single words. To minimise over-interpreting the data, such short prompts were rarely assigned codes such as Specified Consequences and instead were instead typically coded as Reasons (e.g., “marriage”) and Strategies (e.g., “1 glass”). Even prompts that were not single words could often be very short with little to no context, thus limiting interpretation. For example, “Child-free weekend ahead! Good work this week” could in principle be coded with Other People – Children given that children are mentioned. This code was ultimately not applied in this case, however, since children did not seem to be a Reason to change. This prompt was instead coded with Pride/Self-belief (due to the self-congratulation in the second sentence) and no assumption was made about the section pertaining to children. Taken together, these aspects highlight that – despite the careful coding procedures used – the challenges inherent in working with this kind of material should be noted as a study limitation.

Finally, relationships between prompt behaviour (being a prompt engager or not, the type of prompts written etc.) and changes in drinking behaviour were not examined here because, although this would be an interesting line of research, the exploratory nature of the present work is not the ideal platform for such an endeavour. This is because the direction of any relationships detected would be ambiguous (does writing a prompt affect alcohol behaviour, or does a change in alcohol behaviour affect the type of prompt written), and any number of unmeasured variables could lead to erroneous inference. Instead, an experimental approach where individuals are randomized to produce prompts of varying themes/types would be more appropriate to investigate this, so that other factors could be adequately controlled for (e.g., demographics, baseline drinking behaviour etc.)

Conclusions

Offering a module in a digital alcohol intervention where individuals could author their own prompts was appealing for some individuals, in particular women, older individuals, and those for whom reducing drinking was important and who felt they had the knowledge for how to do so. Qualitative evaluation of the content of these self-authored prompts suggests that the majority of respondents were engaging with the information provided via the support tool in an active way, with evidence of goal setting and making plans to change their drinking behaviour. Individuals also used this opportunity to remind themselves of specific and personal reasons they wanted to change their drinking, as well as to encourage themselves to do so.

Data availability

De-identified datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study will be made available upon reasonable request to the corresponding author, after approval of a proposal and with a signed data access agreement.

Abbreviations

RCT:

Randomised Control Trial

IRR:

Incidence Rate Ratio

References

  1. Wood AM, Kaptoge S, Butterworth AS, Willeit P, Warnakula S, Bolton T, et al. Risk thresholds for alcohol consumption: combined analysis of individual-participant data for 599 912 current drinkers in 83 prospective studies. The Lancet. 2018;391(10129):1513–23.

  2. Stanesby O, Callinan S, Graham K, Wilson IM, Greenfield TK, Wilsnack SC, et al. Harm from known others’ drinking by relationship proximity to the harmful drinker and gender: a meta-analysis across 10 countries. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2018;42(9):1693–703.

  3. Shorter GW, Campbell KB, Miller NM, Epton T, O’Hara L, Millen S, et al. Few interventions support the affected other on their own: A systematic review of individual level psychosocial interventions to support those harmed by others’ alcohol use. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2023:1–21.

  4. O'Donnell A, Anderson P, Newbury-Birch D, Schulte B, Schmidt C, Reimer J, et al. The impact of brief alcohol interventions in primary healthcare: a systematic review of reviews. Alcohol and alcoholism. 2014;49(1):66–78.

  5. Bendtsen M, McCambridge J. Reducing alcohol consumption among risky drinkers in the general population of Sweden using an interactive mobile health intervention: protocol for a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Res Protocols. 2019;8(4):e13119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Bendtsen M, McCambridge J, Åsberg K, Bendtsen P. Text messaging interventions for reducing alcohol consumption among risky drinkers: systematic review and meta-analysis. Addiction. 2021;116(5):1021–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Kaner EF, Beyer FR, Garnett C, Crane D, Brown J, Muirhead C et al. Personalised digital interventions for reducing hazardous and harmful alcohol consumption in community-dwelling populations. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2017(9).

  8. Boumparis N, Schulte MH, Riper H. Digital mental health for alcohol and substance use disorders. Curr Treat Options Psychiatry. 2019;6:352–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Crane D, Garnett C, Michie S, West R, Brown J. A smartphone app to reduce excessive alcohol consumption: identifying the effectiveness of intervention components in a factorial randomised control trial. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Bendtsen M, Åsberg K, McCambridge J. Effectiveness of a digital intervention versus alcohol information for online help-seekers in Sweden: a randomised controlled trial. BMC Med. 2022;20(1):176.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Michie S, Richardson M, Johnston M, Abraham C, Francis J, Hardeman W, et al. The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Ann Behav Med. 2013;46(1):81–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Bendtsen M, Gunnarsson KU, McCambridge J. Effects of a waiting list control design on alcohol consumption among online help-seekers: protocol for a randomised controlled trial. BMJ open. 2021;11(8):e049810.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Bendtsen M, Åsberg K, McCambridge J. Mediators of effects of a digital alcohol intervention for online help-seekers: findings from an effectiveness trial. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2023:110957.

  14. Müssener U, Thomas K, Linderoth C, Leijon M, Bendtsen M. A text message–based intervention targeting alcohol consumption among University students: user satisfaction and acceptability study. JMIR Hum Factors. 2018;5(3):e9641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Knox J, Hasin DS, Larson FR, Kranzler HR. Prevention, screening, and treatment for heavy drinking and Alcohol Use Disorder. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(12):1054–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Gunnarsson KU, McCambridge J, Bendtsen M. Reactions to being allocated to a waiting list control group in a digital alcohol intervention trial. Patient Educ Couns. 2023;107:107572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Cunningham JA, Kypri K, McCambridge J. Exploratory randomized controlled trial evaluating the impact of a waiting list control design. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2013;13(1):1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Vaismoradi M, Turunen H, Bondas T. Content analysis and thematic analysis: implications for conducting a qualitative descriptive study. Nurs Health Sci. 2013;15(3):398–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Huang R, RQDA. R-based qualitative data analysis. R package version 0.2-7. Computer software Retrieved from http://rqda r-forge r-project org. 2014.

  20. McElreath R. Statistical rethinking: a bayesian course with examples in R and Stan. CRC press; 2020.

  21. Cook S, Heather N, McCambridge J. Posttreatment motivation and alcohol treatment outcome 9 months later: findings from structural equation modeling. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2015;83(1):232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Jonas K, Diehl M, Brömer P. Effects of attitudinal ambivalence on information processing and attitude-intention consistency. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1997;33(2):190–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Clark JK, Wegener DT, Fabrigar LR. Attitudinal ambivalence and message-based persuasion: motivated processing of proattitudinal information and avoidance of counterattitudinal information. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2008;34(4):565–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Collymore NN, McDermott MR. Evaluating the effects of six alcohol-related message frames on emotions and intentions: the neglected role of disgust. J Health Psychol. 2016;21(9):1907–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Davies EL, Law C, Hennelly SE. You have to be prepared to drink: students’ views about reducing excessive alcohol consumption at university. Health Educ. 2018.

  26. de Visser RO, Smith JA. Young men’s ambivalence toward alcohol. Soc Sci Med. 2007;64(2):350–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Crawford J, Jones A, Rose A, Cooke R. You see the pictures the morning after and you’re like I wish I was in them’: an interpretative phenomenological analysis of university student’s alcohol-related regrets. Psychol Health. 2022;37(4):490–506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. MacNeill V, Foley M, Quirk A, McCambridge J. Shedding light on research participation effects in behaviour change trials: a qualitative study examining research participant experiences. BMC Public Health. 2016;16:1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Haug S, Schaub MP, Venzin V, Meyer C, John U, Gmel G. A pre-post study on the appropriateness and effectiveness of a web-and text messaging-based intervention to reduce problem drinking in emerging adults. J Med Internet Res. 2013;15(9):e2755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Andrews B, Klein B, Van Nguyen H, Corboy D, McLaren S, Watson S. Efficacy of a digital mental health biopsychosocial transdiagnostic intervention with or without therapist-assistance for adults with anxiety and depression: an adaptive randomised clinical trial. J Med Internet Res (Forthcoming). 2023:45135.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Open access funding provided by Linköping University. This study was conducted under the auspices of the Alcohol Research Council of the Swedish Alcohol Retailing Monopoly (grant numbers 2019-0056 and 2020-0043) and the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life, and Welfare (grant number 2022 − 00193). The funders had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report.

Open access funding provided by Linköping University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Elizabeth S. Collier: Methodology. Formal analysis. Visualisation. Writing – original draft. Writing – review & editing. Jenny Blomqvist: Formal analysis. Jim McCambridge: Conceptualisation. Writing – review & editing. Joel Crawford: Writing – review & editing. Marcus Bendtsen: Conceptualisation. Investigation. Validation. Project administration. Writing – review & editing. Funding acquisition. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elizabeth S. Collier.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval for the study was received on 2018-06-11 by the Regional Ethical Committee in Linköping, Sweden (Dnr 2018/417 − 31). All individuals provided informed consent prior to randomization.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

MB owns a private company (Alexit AB) that maintains and distributes evidence-based lifestyle interventions to be used by the public and in health care settings. Alexit AB played no role in developing the intervention, study design, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of this report. Services developed and maintained by Alexit AB were used for sending text messages and data collection. ESC, JB, JC, and JM declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Collier, E.S., Blomqvist, J., Crawford, J. et al. Exploratory mixed methods analysis of self-authored content from participants in a digital alcohol intervention trial. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy 18, 60 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13011-023-00569-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13011-023-00569-4

Keywords