Skip to main content

Effects of perceived cocaine availability on subjective and objective responses to the drug

Abstract

Rationale

Several lines of evidence suggest that cocaine expectancy and craving are two related phenomena. The present study assessed this potential link by contrasting reactions to varying degrees of the drug's perceived availability.

Method

Non-treatment seeking individuals with cocaine dependence were administered an intravenous bolus of cocaine (0.2 mg/kg) under 100% ('unblinded'; N = 33) and 33% ('blinded'; N = 12) probability conditions for the delivery of drug. Subjective ratings of craving, high, rush and low along with heart rate and blood pressure measurements were collected at baseline and every minute for 20 minutes following the infusions.

Results

Compared to the 'blinded' subjects, their 'unblinded' counterparts had similar craving scores on a multidimensional assessment several hours before the infusion, but reported higher craving levels on a more proximal evaluation, immediately prior to the receipt of cocaine. Furthermore, the 'unblinded' subjects displayed a more rapid onset of high and rush cocaine responses along with significantly higher cocaine-induced heart rate elevations.

Conclusion

These results support the hypothesis that cocaine expectancy modulates subjective and objective responses to the drug. Provided the important public health policy implications of heavy cocaine use, health policy makers and clinicians alike may favor cocaine craving assessments performed in the settings with access to the drug rather than in more neutral environments as a more meaningful marker of disease staging and assignment to the proper level of care.

1. Background

Cocaine dependence is a chronically relapsing disorder leading to a variety of medical complications along with devastating psychosocial consequences. It remains a major public health problem bearing enormous societal costs and is currently afflicting over 1.5 million American citizens [1]. Thus, epidemiological data presented at the recent Community Epidemiology Work Group meeting indicate that in a number of major US cities, representing 21 geographic areas, hospital admissions for primary cocaine-related problems exceeded those for heroin, methamphetamine and marihuana, altogether [2].

Craving, defined as an intense and often irrepressible urge to seek and consume the drug, resulting in relapse even after extended periods of abstinence [3], is one of the most malignant and treatment resistant features of cocaine dependence [4]. Therefore, in the search for innovative and efficient therapeutic approaches, craving has been subjected to an intensive scientific inquiry in both laboratory and clinical arenas. One consistent finding produced by this effort, is a considerable overlap between neural correlates of craving, cognition and motivation [3, 5–7]. Hence, understanding how cocaine produces its effects necessitates investigation of the cognitive and motivational contexts of craving.

Cocaine craving may result from the desire for pleasurable effects of the drug [8]. A closely related concept to this model is that of dopamine as the motivation/cognition-related neurotransmitter, the phasic release of which underlies the anticipation of pleasurable outcomes and motivational behaviors targeted at avoiding the loss of pleasure [9–16].

Primate electrophysiological studies of individual dopamine neurons in the brain [9, 17–21] have begun dissecting these motivational processes that could be potentially broadened to cocaine craving. Neuronal responses to reward and reward-predicating stimuli appear to be dependent on event predictability i.e., neuronal activity is increased by events with reward values which are better than predicted, is uninfluenced by events that are as good as predicted, and is reduced by events that are worse than predicted [13, 22, 23]. Elevated dopamine concentrations may also prime the organism to associate cues around cocaine use with the pleasure experienced when drug is taken [12, 13, 15, 18–20, 24]. When these cues are encountered again, they may elicit a surge of activity in dopaminergic neurons leading to exaggerated anticipatory craving of the future cocaine reward [25]. However, if individuals with cocaine dependence are exposed to stimuli associated with cocaine when they cannot consume it, they may experience a decrease in dopamine activity, which may potentially lead to a dysphoric state of withdrawal [16].

These observations lead Volkow and colleagues [16] to assume the existence of specific neural loci related to the processes of expectancy, predictions of reward and contingency assessment. They assessed this entity by using a two-by-two within subjects design to measure the effects of expectation on subjective effects and regional brain metabolism in cocaine abusers under four conditions: 1) expecting and receiving placebo; 2) expecting placebo and receiving a drug similar to cocaine, methylphenidate; 3) expecting methylphenidate and receiving placebo; 4) expecting and receiving methylphenidate. The authors found that expectation of the drug and subsequent receipt of the drug led to an increased physiological response. This was reflected in a 50% increase in global brain glucose utilization, with specific increases in the thalamic and cerebellar regions and an enhancement in subjective assessments ("drug liking," "high," "feel drug," and "restlessness") compared to expectation of placebo and receipt of drug [16]. Thus, after methylphenidate infusion, the presence or absence of the drug-induced subjective effects apparently involved some retrieval and comparison to the "remembered utility" [16], or the emotional memory associated with the prior use.

The present study attempted to extend Volkow et al.'s [16] findings to cocaine itself and to craving. To that end, cocaine dependent subjects' self-reports of craving when they were certain in the receipt of the drug were contrasted to those when such probability stood at only 33%. The primary hypothesis to be tested was that subjects experience stronger craving when they know that cocaine infusion is forthcoming versus a situation in which it is less likely. Other subjective ratings i.e., high, rush and low [16, 26] were assessed using exploratory analysis. Finally, given the previous finding by our group that subjective expectancy ratings accounted for some variance in the hemodynamic responses to 'unblinded' cocaine [27], we also examined the effects of perceived cocaine availability on heart rate and on mean arterial pressure (MAP).

2. Methods

2.1 Participants

The study samples were comprised of non-treatment seeking cocaine-dependent participants recruited by advertisement who received cocaine under 'unblinded' (N = 33; 28 males and 5 females, 23 Caucasian and 10 African-American; mean age ± SD: 34.2 ± 6.0 years; weight: 77.0 ± 15.2 kg) and 'blinded' conditions (9 males and 3 females; 4 Caucasian and 8 African-American; mean age: 39.7 ± 7.8 years; weight: 79.9 ± 13.6 kg). The subjects were in good physical health, not taking prescription medications or other illegal drugs. All met DSM-IV criteria for cocaine dependence with no past or current major depression or other Axis I psychiatric diagnosis as assessed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID; [28]). Individuals using prescription medications or those with dependence on other illegal drugs or alcohol were excluded from study participation. 'Unblinded' and 'blinded' subjects' cocaine use averaged 13.2 ± 7.0 and 6.5 ± 7.4 (t43 = 0.13, p = 0.9) times per month with the last cocaine use 1.7 ± 1.2 and 1.4 ± 1.1 (t43 = 0.76, p = 0.45) days prior to study participation, respectively, with smoking being the primary route of cocaine ingestion. Subjects provided written informed consent for the Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) Institutional Review Board-approved protocol prior to initiating the study. The data from these samples were previously reported [29–31] and are included here for the purpose of contrasting the two expectancy states (i.e., 'unblinded' vs. 'blinded').

2.2 Clinical protocol

Subjects were admitted to the MGH General Clinical Research Center, having abstained from cocaine for at least 10 hours and completed medical workup, including urinalysis and breathalyzer tests for drug use, and structured clinical assessments including SCID [28], Addiction Severity Index (ASI; [32]) and a multidimensional craving questionnaire [33, 34].

The latter assessment tool was demonstrated to be a reliable predictor of short-, but not long-term cocaine use [33, 34] and it measures various aspects of craving on a Likert-type scale (items rated on a scale of 0–9), including (i) current intensity; (ii) desire to avoid using; (iii) capacity to resist using; (iv) responsiveness to drug-related conditioned stimuli; and (v) imagined likelihood of use if in a setting with access to drugs. Subjects were assessed for spontaneous craving after at least 10 hours abstinence prior to administration of the infusions. Total spontaneous craving score was derived by adding together ratings scores on items 1, 4 and 5 and subtracting items 2 and 3 [35]. The 'unblinded' and 'blinded' groups were comparable in terms of their ASI drug composite scores (0.2 ± 0.1 and 0.2 ± 0.1; t43 = 0.2, p = 0.9) and the craving ratings the total scores (17.6 ± 10.3 and 16.6 ± 10.5, respectively; t43 = 0.3, p = 0.8) on the multidimensional craving questionnaire.

For the 'unblinded' condition, the subjects were informed that they would receive 0.2 mg/kg of cocaine, whereas the 'blinded' subjects were told they could receive any of three infusions, namely cocaine 0.2 mg/kg, a low dose of hydrocortisone or a placebo and that each subsequent infusion was non-contingent on the experience with the one preceding it.

Bilateral antecubital intravenous catheters were placed (left forearm for infusions, right forearm for serial blood sampling) and the infusion(s) was/were commenced after a 60-minute rest period. Cocaine, hydrocortisone and saline were administered as intravenous boluses in a volume of 10 mL, 2 hours apart in a double-blind, randomized and counterbalanced cross-over design. Only cocaine data are included in this report; hydrocortisone and saline results are presented elsewhere [29–31]. Continuous hemodynamic monitoring was performed using OmniTrak 3100 patient monitoring system (Orlando, FL) and a board-certified cardiologist was present throughout the whole study.

2.3 Subjective self-reports

The individualized description of subjective responses were categorized into 4 components : craving, high, rush, and low [36]. Cocaine craving was defined proactively with each subject; clinically, as an urge to use the drug and operationally, in terms of the action the individual would be willing to engage in order to get more cocaine. The following were subjectively defined, but not necessarily associated with a behavioral response or with the planning of physical activity: high (well-being, self-confidence, and sociability), rush (perception of elevated heart rate and sweating, along with sensations of "speeding") and low (dysphoric affect distinct from high experience diminishment) [36]. Thus, of the four measures, only craving was associated with a behavioral response or with planning or implementation of physical activity. Therefore, by definition, only craving self-report could be defined as a motivational state [36]. Behavioral ratings were acquired using a Macintosh laptop placed on a table in front of the subject. The words "CRAVING", "HIGH", "RUSH" and "LOW" appeared sequentially on the screen and with each appearance the subject used the keyboard to rate them on a continuous Likert-type scale of 0 (none) to 3 (extreme). Ratings were initiated 2 minutes pre-infusion and a full set was collected once per minute until 20 minutes post-infusion.

2.4 Data analysis

Data were analyzed using SPSS 13.0 for Mac (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL). Baseline subjective scores were determined from the mean of the self-reported ratings at 1- and 2-minute time points prior to cocaine administration and were compared using Student's t-test. When variances were determined to be dissimilar, t-test corrected for separate variances was used. Repeated measures ANOVAs – 2 (group: 'unblinded' and 'blinded') × 20 (time: 20 minutes) were used to assess the subjective- and hemodynamic variables. When violations of sphericity/homogeneity assumptions were determined, a stringent Greenhouse-Geisser (GG) correction was applied. In light of previously reported cocaine's subjective effects in both groups (Elman et al., 2002; Elman et al., 2003), a priori emphasis was given to the group by time interaction effect comparing responses of the 'unblinded' and 'blinded' groups. Data were summarized as Mean ± standard deviation. All analyses were two-tailed with α < 0.05 set as the threshold for statistical significance.

3. Results

3.1 Covariation

Subjects' age was used as a covariate because participants in the 'unblinded' group were younger (t43 = 2.47, p < 0.02) than in the 'blinded' group. To adjust for variability in the baseline measures, subjective and hemodynamic data were also (in addition to age) covaried for the pre-infusion baseline values, as appropriate (e.g., baseline craving reports for the craving data).

3.2 Subjective responses

Subjective data are graphically presented in Figure 1. Despite similar craving scores on the multidimensional craving questionnaire, pre-infusion craving ratings were significantly higher (t43 = 2.05, corrected p < 0.009) in the 'unblinded' subjects, (0.67 ± 0.9), as compared to their 'blinded' counterparts (0.13 ± 0.4). In contrast, there were no significant group differences in the pre-infusion high, rush, or low ratings (p > 0.35).

Figure 1
figure 1

Subjective responses. Effects of 'unblinded' vs. 'blinded' cocaine administration on craving, high rush and low ratings. Data are presented as mean (± SEM).

For craving, repeated-measures ANOVA, covaried for the baseline self-rating scores and age, revealed no effect of group or group by time interaction (F1,41 = 1.8, p > 0.20), indicating that following cocaine administration, both groups' relative craving levels increased in a similar fashion across the study time frame, with a main effect for time (F19,760 = 3.21, GG adjusted p < 0.02).

For high, the group by time interaction was significant (F19,760 = 2.09, p < 0.004), this effect, however, was not sustained with the GG correction (adjusted p = 0.06). Rush ratings differences reached statistical significance resulting in significant group by time interaction (F19,760 = 3.37, GG adjusted p < 0.004). Group differences in the low ratings did not reach statistical significance (F19,760 = 0.55, GG adjusted p = 0.73).

3.3 Hemodynamic responses

Cocaine produced significantly higher heart rate increases in the 'unblinded' subjects (Figure 2) (group effect: F1,41 = 7.26, p < 0.01; group-by-time interaction: F19,779 = 3.25, GG adjusted p < 0.006; mean change from baseline: 30% versus 20%). Changes in MAP were not apparent across time by group (F19,779 = 1.63, GG adjusted p = 0.12).

Figure 2
figure 2

Heart rate. Effects of 'unblinded' vs. 'blinded' cocaine administration on mean arterial pressure and heart rate. Data are presented as mean (± SEM).

4. Discussion

The major finding of this study is that changing the motivational context from 100% to 33% cocaine expectancy, by matching a potential cocaine infusion to two other potential infusions, significantly modified subjective and objective cocaine responses. To our knowledge, this is the first report of a non-pharmacological effect of perceived cocaine availability on self-reported baseline craving prior to receiving cocaine and on subjective and hemodynamic variables following cocaine administration.

The elevated high ratings in the 'unblinded' group, although did not survive stringent statistical adjustment, are suggestive of an expectancy component in cocaine's rewarding effects and are consistent with the greater "high" reports in the context of 100% vs. 50% methylphenidate expectancy state [37]. Although there were important methodological similarities between Volkow et al. [37] and our study (i.e., enrollment of cocaine-dependent subjects, use of subjective self-ratings and acute administration of a psychostimulant), there were also substantial differences in specific definitions of self-ratings, the priming agent, level of uncertainty about the receipt of the drug, intersubject design, and the measures of hemodynamic responses. Thus, these independent results strongly support the validity of the relationship between expectancy context and cocaine effects reflected in self-reports and in physiological changes.

It is notable that notwithstanding similar (to the 'blinded' group) craving scores on a multidimensional assessment several hours before the infusions, 'unblinded' subjects reported higher craving levels on a more proximal evaluation, immediately prior to the infusions. These data are consistent with a substantial body of literature [38–40] documenting failure to detect a relationship between measures of craving obtained in hospital and laboratory environments (where expectancy of obtaining drugs is low) and subsequent relapse to seeking and consuming cocaine. Thus, this study suggests that even within a laboratory environment, craving can be exacerbated by the imminent availability of the drug. Furthermore, our data may be indicative that the effect of expectancy is time sensitive such that proximal rather than distant prospects of getting the drug have greater effect on craving. Therefore, in order to draw valid conclusions about clinical outcomes, laboratory-based research may need to take into account motivational and cognitive contexts of craving e.g., perceived availability of the drug.

Probability of 100% for receipt of cocaine obviously presents a stronger drug-related cue, as compared to 33%, for the sensitized dopaminergic system, thus evoking heightened anticipatory craving. This difference in craving response levels off because the next cue i.e., the priming dose of cocaine itself is not different between the groups. Given the intuitively obvious liking of the desired objects [41], subjects with elevated craving for cocaine, as those in the 'unblinded' condition, presented heightened hedonic rush responses. Alternatively, these may be reflective of the hemodynamic effects (e.g., perception of the elevated heart rate).

Subjective and hemodynamic cocaine effects may indeed be connected via conditioned mechanisms. Thus, heart rate elevations repeatedly paired with cocaine use may become a conditioned stimulus and elicit cocaine-like psychological responses [42, 43], which in turn may produce increases in cardiovascular activity [44]. Such a self-sustaining feed-forward loop whereby minor stimuli can trigger escalating reactions may be implicated in both, the chronically deteriorating course of cocaine dependence [45] and in high rates of cardio- and cerebrovascular morbidity associated with this illness [46]. Notably, that causality does not run in the opposite direction as pharmacologically induced decrements in hemodynamic indices do not seem to affect subjective effects of cocaine [47, 48].

Homeostatic regulation of hemodynamic function can be dissected into distinct, but interacting catecholaminergic systems including dopamine, norepinephrine and epinephrine. Dopamine has greater β than α affinity, which explains its predominant heart rate effects. On the other hand, a rather selective α agonist, norepinephrine, primarily increases blood pressure, accompanied by only minimal heart rate changes. Epinephrine binds both α and β adrenoceptors, thus evenly affecting blood pressure and heart rate [49]. Hence, different slopes of heart rate increases in the 'unblinded' subjects may reflect an expectancy-induced surge in dopamine [50, 51], which is the key neurochemical implicated in expectancy [13, 19, 20, 52] and in sensitization [53] effects.

An alternative/complimentary explanation may be consistent with an idea that a psychological state, such as cocaine expectancy, could alter the homeostatic baroreflex, which is responsible for slowing the heart rate during increases in blood pressure by relaying neural stimuli generated by the arterial receptors' distortion to the CNS, nucleus of the solitary tract [49, 54]. Future research can evaluate signs of diminished baroreceptor function such as orthostatic hypotension by performing supine and upright blood pressure measurements along with Valsalva maneuver and plasma catecholamine assays.

This study's strengths include fully randomized and counterbalanced cocaine administration and employment of both subjective and objective measures of cocaine response. There are, however, some caveats that have to be considered in interpreting the present data. First, a limitation of the present study is that group differences in age required us to perform statistical adjustments for this potentially confounding variable. Second, the lack of true randomization into the 'unblinded' and 'blinded' groups, along with the modest size of the latter, render our results preliminary pending replication in more rigorously designed trials.

Third, although the crossover design in the 'blinded' group offers certain advantages [55, 56], a few effects could have carried over from the hydrocortisone session. This is unlikely, though, given only transient and subtle subjective effects of hydrocortisone [34] and similar baseline values of the reported variables (excluding craving). Nonetheless, to fully account for such confounding, a second placebo arm could be an important consideration for future research. Fourth, mostly men participated in the experiment and results may not be easily extrapolated to women, as gender differences in craving have been previously demonstrated [57]. Finally, to accommodate for motivational and attentional deficits commonly encountered in cocaine dependent populations [8, 58–61], this study focused on short-term acute subjective responses. As some of the self-ratings did not return to baseline at the conclusion of the 20-minute data collection period, longer study duration may have yielded different results.

5. Conclusion

In conclusion, rather than being a unitary state characterized by one set of motivations [62], cocaine craving is modulated by its own availability, which also affects other emotions evoked by the drug and cocaine-induced hemodynamic changes.

The inability to correlate craving and subsequent drug taking behavior, as has been shown for laboratory settings, potentially generalizes to hospital and clinical treatment facilities [34, 63]. A better model could be evaluations performed at a site with a greater likelihood of obtaining the drug. Thus, current results suggest that more ecologically valid assessments of craving, such as phone interviews in a patient's home, might be important for understanding the patterns of craving that lead to drug use and subsequently for intervention and prevention techniques as well as for proper disease staging and treatment matching procedures employed in public health policy research and determination [64, 65].

References

  1. SAMHSA: Results from the 2005 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (Office of Applied Studies). NSDUH Series H-30, DHHS Publication No SMA 06-4194. http://oas.samhsa.gov/NSDUH/2k5NSDUH/2k5results.htm#Ch2

  2. CEWG: Epidemiological Trends in Drug Abuse: Advance Report. Epidemiological Trends in Drug Abuse. 2006, Bethesda , National Institutes of Health, Division of Epidemiology, Services and Prevention Research, National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2006 (Nov. 20, 2006): 21-24.

  3. Kalivas PW, Volkow ND: The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry. 2005, 162: 1403-1413. 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Kreek MJ: Goals and rationale for pharmacotherapeutic approach in treating cocaine dependence: insights from basic clinical research. NIDA Res Monogr. 1998, National Institute on Drug Abuse, 175: 5-35.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Grant S, London ED, Newlin DB, Villemangne VL, Liu X, Contoreggi C, Phillips RL, Kimes AS, Margolin A: Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996, 93: 12040-12045. 10.1073/pnas.93.21.12040.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Kilts CD, Schweitzer JB, Quinn CK, Gross RE, Faber TL, Muhammad F, Ely TD, Hoffman JM, Drexler KPG: Neural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001, 58 (4): 334-341. 10.1001/archpsyc.58.4.334.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Volkow ND, Fowler JS: Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000, 10 (3): 318-325. 10.1093/cercor/10.3.318.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND: Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2002, 159: 1642-1652. 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Mirenowicz J, Schultz W: Preferential activation of midbrain dopamine neurons by appetitive rather than aversive stimuli. Nature. 1996, 379 (6564): 449-451. 10.1038/379449a0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Montague PR, Hyman SE, Cohen JD: Computational roles for dopamine in behavioral control. Nature. 2004, 431 (7010): 760-767. 10.1038/nature03015.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Pessiglione M, Seymour B, Flandinm G, Dolan RJ, Frith CD: Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature. 2006, 442 (7106): 1042-1045. 10.1038/nature05051.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Phillips PEM, Stuber GD, Heien MLAV, Wightman RM, Carelli RM: Subsecond dopamine release promotes cocaine seeking. Nature. 2003, 422 (6932): 614-618. 10.1038/nature01476.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Schultz W: Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1998, 80: 1-27.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Stuber GD, Wightman RM, Carelli RM: Extinction of cocaine self-administration reveals functionally and temporally distinct dopaminergic signals in the nucleus accumbens. Neuron. 2005, 46: 661-669. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.036.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Sutton MA, Beninger RJ: Psychopharmacology of conditioned reward: evidence for a rewarding signal at D1-like dopamine receptors. Psychopharmacology. 1999, 144 (2): 95-110. 10.1007/s002130050982.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, Jayne M, Ma Y, Wong C: Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci. 2006, 26 (24): 6583-6588. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1544-06.2006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Fiorillo CD, Tobler PN, Schultz W: Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science. 2003, 299: 1898-1902. 10.1126/science.1077349.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Ljungberg T, Apicella P, Schultz W: Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions. J Neurophysiol. 1992, 67 (1): 145-163.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Schultz W: Reward signaling by dopamine neurons. Neuroscientist. 2001, 7 (4): 293-302.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Schultz W, Apicella P, Ljungberg T: Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task. J Neurosci. 1993, 13 (3): 900-913.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Tobler PN, Fiorillo CD, Schultz W: Adaptive coding of reward value by dopamine neurons. Science. 2005, 307 (5715): 1642-1645. 10.1126/science.1105370.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR: A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997, 275: 1593-1598. 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Schultz W: Neural coding of basic reward terms of animal learning theory, game theory, microeconomics and behavioral ecology. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004, 14 (2): 139-147. 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.017.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Duvauchelle CL, Ikegami A, Asami S, Robens J, Kressin K, Castaneda E: Effects of cocaine context on NAcc dopamine and behavioral activity after repeated intravenous cocaine administration. Brain Res. 2000, 862 (1-2): 49-58. 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02091-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Redish AD: Addiction as a computational process gone awry. Science. 2004, 306 (5703):

  26. Elman I, Breiter HC, Gollub RL, Krause S, Kantor HL, Baumgartner WA, Gastfriend DR, Rosen BR: Depressive symptomatology and cocaine-induced pituitary-adrenal axis activation in individuals with cocaine dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1999, 56 (1): 39-45. 10.1016/S0376-8716(99)00009-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Lundahl LH, Lukas SE: Negative cocaine effect expectancies are associated with subjective response to cocaine challenge in recreational cocaine users. Addict Behav. 2006, 32 (6): 1262-1271. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.09.001.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. 1997, Washington, DC , American Psychiatric Press

    Google Scholar 

  29. Elman I, Karlsgodt KH, Gastfriend DR, Chabris CF, Breiter HC: Cocaine-primed craving and its relationship to depressive symptomatology in individuals with cocaine dependence. J Psychopharmacol. 2002, 16 (2): 163-167.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Elman I, Lukas SE, Karlsgodt KH, Gasic GP, Breiter HC: Acute cortisol administration triggers craving in individuals with cocaine dependence. Psychopharmacol Bull. 2003, 37 (3): 84-89.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Hopper JW, Karlsgodt KH, Adler CM, Macklin EA, Lukas SE, Elman I: Effects of acute cortisol and cocaine administration on attention, recall and recognition task performance in individuals with cocaine dependence. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2004, 19 (7): 511-516. 10.1002/hup.616.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. McLellan AT, Kushner H, Metzger D, Peters R, Smith I, Grissom G, Pettinati H, Argeriou M: The fifth edn. of the Addiction Severity Index. J Subst Abuse Treat. 1992, 9: 199-213. 10.1016/0740-5472(92)90062-S.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Weiss RD, Griffin ML, Hufford C: Craving in hospitalized cocaine abusers as a predictor of outcome. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 1995, 21 (3): 289-301.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Weiss RD, Griffin ML, Hufford C, Muenz LR, Najavits LM, Jansson SB, Kogan J, Thompson HJ: Early prediction of initiation of abstinence from cocaine. Use of a craving questionnaire. Am J Addict. 1997, 6 (3): 224-231.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Elman I, Karlsgodt KH, Gastfriend DR: Gender differences in cocaine craving among non-treatment seeking individuals with cocaine dependence. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2001, 27 (2): 193-202. 10.1081/ADA-100103705.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Breiter HC, Gollub RL, Weisskoff RM, Kennedy DN, Makris N, Berke JD, Goodman JM, Kantor HL, Gastfriend DR, Riorden JP, Mathew RT, Rosen BR, Hyman SE: Acute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion. Neuron. 1997, 19 (3): 591-611. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80374-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Ma Y, Fowler JS, Zhu W, Maynard L, Telang F, Vaska P, Ding YS, Wong C, Swanson JM: Expectation enhances the regional brain metabolic and reinforcing effects of stimulants in cocaine abusers. J Neurosci. 2003, 23 (36): 11461-11468.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Avants SK, Margolin A, Kosten TR, Cooney NL: Differences between responders and nonresponders to cocaine cues in the laboratory. Addict Behav. 1995, 20 (2): 215-224. 10.1016/0306-4603(94)00066-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Dudish-Poulsen SA, Hatsukami DK: Dissociation between subjective and behavioral responses after cocaine stimuli presentations. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1997, 47 (1): 1-9. 10.1016/S0376-8716(97)00054-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Rohsenow DJ, Niaura RS, Childress AR, Abrams DB, Monti PM: Cue reactivity in addictive behaviors: theoretical and treatment implications. Int J Addict. 1990, 25 (7A-8A): 957-993.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Berridge KC, Robinson TE: Parsing Reward. Trends Neurosci. 2003, 26 (9): 507-513. 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. O'Brien CP, Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman RN: Classical conditioning in drug-dependent humans. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1992, 654: 400-415. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb25984.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Childress AR, Ehrman RN, McLellan AT, O'Brien CP: Conditioned craving and arousal in cocaine addiction: a preliminary report. NIDA Res Monogr. 1988, 81: 74-80.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Sinha R, Talih M, Malison R, Kreek MJ: Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and sympatho-adreno-medullary responses during stress-induced and drug cue-induced cocaine craving states. Psychopharmacology. 2003, 170 (1): 62-72. 10.1007/s00213-003-1525-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Weiss F, Ciccocioppo R, Parsons LH, Katner S, Liu X, Zorrilla EP, Valdez GR, Ben-Shahar O, Angeletti S, Richter RR: Compulsive drug-seeking behavior and relapse. Neuroadaptation, stress, and conditioning factors. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2001, 937: 1-26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Darke S, Kaye S, Duflou J: Cocaine-related fatalities in New South Wales, Australia 1993-2002. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005, 77 (2): 107-114. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.07.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Sofuoglu M, Brown S, Babb DA, Pentel PR, Hatsukami DK: Effects of Labetalol Treatment on the Physiological and Subjective Response to Smoked Cocaine. Pharmacol Biochemi Behav. 2000, 65 (2): 255-259. 10.1016/S0091-3057(99)00201-4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Sofuoglu M, Brown S, Babb DA, Pentel PR, Hatsukami DK: Carvedilol affects the physiological and behavioral response to smoked cocaine in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2000, 60 (1): 69-10.1016/S0376-8716(99)00143-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Goldstein DS: Stress, Catecholamines, and Cardiovascular Disease. 1995, New York , Oxford University Press, 539-

    Google Scholar 

  50. Faraj BA, Camp VM, Davis DC, Kutner M, Cotsonis GA, Holloway T: Elevated concentrations of dopamine sulfate in plasma of cocaine abusers. Biochem Pharmacol. 1993, 46 (8): 1453-1457. 10.1016/0006-2952(93)90111-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Sofuoglu M, Nelson D, Babb DA, Hatsukami DK: Intravenous cocaine increases plasma epinephrine and norepinephrine in humans. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2001, 68 (3): 455-459. 10.1016/S0091-3057(01)00482-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Schultz W: Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2000, 1: 199-207. 10.1038/35044563.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Saloman L, Lanteri C, Glowinski J, Tassin JP: Behavioral sensitization to amphetamine results from an uncoupling between noradrenergic and serotonergic neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006, 103 (19): 7476-7481. 10.1073/pnas.0600839103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Sleight P, Fox P, Lopez R, Brooks DE: The effect of mental arithmetic on blood pressure variability and baroreflex sensitivity in man. Clin Sci Mol Med Suppl. 1978, 4: 381s-382s.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Hedayat AS, Stufken J: Optimal and efficient crossover designs under different assumptions about the carryover effects. J Biopharm Stat. 2003, 13 (3): 519-528. 10.1081/BIP-120022771.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Prescott P: Construction of uniform-balanced cross-over designs for any odd number of treatments. Stat Med. 1999, 18 (3): 265-272. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0258(19990215)18:3<265::AID-SIM13>3.0.CO;2-Q.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Elman I, Karlsgodt KH, Gastfriend DR: Gender differences in cocaine craving among non-treatment-seeking individuals with cocaine dependence. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2001, 27 (2): 193-202. 10.1081/ADA-100103705.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Biggins CA, MacKay S, Clark W, Fein G: Event-related potential evidence for frontal cortex effects of chronic cocaine dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 1997, 42 (6): 472-485. 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00425-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Bolla KI, Cadet JL, London ED: The neuropsychiatry of chronic cocaine abuse. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998, 10: 280-289.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Hester R, Dixon V, Garavan H: A consistent attentional bias for drug-related material in active cocaine users across word and picture versions of the emotional Stroop task. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006, 81 (3): 251-257. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.07.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ: The addicted human brain: insights from imaging studies. J Clin Invest. 2003, 111: 1444-1451. 10.1172/JCI200318533.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Hommer DW: Functional imaging of craving. Alcohol Res Health. 1999, 23 (3): 187-196.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Weiss RD, Griffin ML, Mazurick C, Berkman B, Gastfriend DR, Frank A, Barber JP, Blaine J, Salloum I, Moras K: The relationship between cocaine craving, psychosocial treatment, and subsequent cocaine use. Am J Psychiatry. 2003, 160 (7): 1320-1325. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.7.1320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. McGee MD, Mee-Lee D: Rethinking patient placement: The human service matrix model for matching services to needs. J Subst Abuse Treat. 1997, 14 (2): 141-10.1016/S0740-5472(96)00122-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. McKay JR, Cacciola JS, McLellan AT, Alterman AI, Wirtz PW: An initial evaluation of the psychosocial dimensions of the American Society of Addiction Medicine criteria for inpatient versus intensive outpatient substance abuse rehabilitation . Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1997, 58 (3): 239-252.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants DA014410, DA017959 and DA015036 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse.

Written consent for publication was obtained from the patient or their relative.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rinah T Yamamoto.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

RY carried out the statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript. KK coordinated the study and carried out the procedures. DR contributed cardiovascular expertise and helped drafting the manuscript. SL helped with the conceptualization and interpretation of the findings. IE designed the study and supervised the co-authors in their writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Yamamoto, R.T., Karlsgodt, K.H., Rott, D. et al. Effects of perceived cocaine availability on subjective and objective responses to the drug. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy 2, 30 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1186/1747-597X-2-30

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1747-597X-2-30

Keywords